It’s the final race. Eight athletes are lined up on the track, their feet tensely braced against the starting blocks. They hear the count off: “On Your Marks!”, “Get Set,” and then, a fraction of a second before the gunshot, a runner leaps forward, disqualifying himself from the competition. It is in such moments that a commonly overlooked aspect of behavior—action suppression—painfully comes to light.
A study published today in the journal Nature uncovers how the brain stops us from jumping the gun. “We discovered a brain area responsible for driving action and another for suppressing that drive. We could also trigger impulsive behavior by manipulating neurons in these areas,” said the study’s senior author, Joe Paton, Director of the Champalimaud Neuroscience Program in Portugal.
Solving a riddle
Paton’s team set out to tackle a puzzle that arose in part from Parkinson’s and Huntington’s Disease. These conditions manifest as movement disorders with broadly opposite symptoms. While Huntington’s patients suffer from uncontrolled, involuntary movement, Parkinson’s patients struggle with action initiation. Peculiarly, both conditions stem from dysfunction of the same brain region: the basal ganglia. How can the same structure support contradictory functions?
According to Paton, a valuable hint emerged from past studies, which identified two major circuits in the basal ganglia: the direct and indirect pathways. It is thought that while the activity of the direct pathway promotes movement, the indirect pathway suppresses it. However, the precise manner by which this interplay is carried out was largely unknown.
A timing task with a twist
Paton took an original approach to the problem. Whereas previous studies investigated the basal ganglia during movement, Paton’s team focused on active action suppression instead.
The team designed a task where mice had to determine whether an interval separating two tones was longer or shorter than 1.5 seconds. If it was shorter, a reward would be provided on the left side of the box, and if it was longer, it would be available on the right.
“The key was that the mouse had to stay perfectly still in the period between the two tones,” said Bruno Cruz, a doctoral student in the lab. “So even if the animal was certain the 1.5-second mark had passed, it needed to suppress the urge to move until after the second tone sounded, and only then go for the reward.”
https://youtube.com/watch?v=UjW7E8itxHQ%3Fcolor%3Dwhite
An impulsivity ‘switch’
The researchers tracked neural activity of both pathways while the mouse performed the task. As in past studies, activity levels were similar when the mouse was moving. However, things changed during the action-suppression period.
“Interestingly, unlike the coactivation we and others have observed during movement, activity patterns across the two pathways were strikingly different during the action suppression period. The activity of the indirect pathway was overall higher and it continuously increased while the mouse waited for the second tone,” said Cruz.
According to the authors, this observation suggests that the indirect pathway flexibly supports the behavioral goals of the animal. “As time passes, the mouse becomes more confident that it’s in a ‘long-interval’ trial. And so its urge to move becomes increasingly more difficult to restrain. It’s likely that this continuous increase in activity reflects this internal struggle,” Cruz explained.
Inspired by this idea, Cruz tested the effect of inhibiting the indirect pathway. This manipulation caused the mice to behave impulsively more often, significantly increasing the number of trials where they darted to the reward port prematurely. With this innovative approach, the team effectively uncovered an “impulsivity switch.”
“This discovery has broad implications,” Paton reflected. “In addition to the clear relevance for Parkinson’s and Huntington’s Disease, it also provides a unique opportunity to investigate conditions of impulse control, such as addiction and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder.”
Searching for the drive to act
The team identified a brain region that actively suppresses the drive to act, but where does that drive originate? Since the direct pathway is thought to promote action, the immediate suspect was the direct pathway of the same region. However, the mouse’s behavior was practically unaffected when the researchers inhibited it.
“We knew the mice were experiencing a strong drive to act because removing suppression promoted impulsive-like action. But it wasn’t immediately clear where else the site of action promotion could be. To answer this question, we decided to turn to computational modeling,” Paton recalled.
“Mathematical models are extremely useful for making sense of complex systems, such as this one,” added Gonçalo Guiomar, a doctoral student in the lab. “We took accumulated knowledge about the basal ganglia, formulated it mathematically, and tested how the system processes information. We then combined the model’s prediction with evidence from previous studies and identified a promising new candidate: the dorsomedial striatum.”
The team’s hypothesis was correct. Inhibiting neurons of the direct pathway in this new region was sufficient for altering the mouse’s behavior. “Both regions we recorded from are located in a part of the basal ganglia called the striatum. The first area is in charge of so-called ‘low-level’ motor-sensory functions and the second is dedicated to ‘high-levels’ functions such as decision-making,” Guiomar explained.
From action to temptation and beyond
The authors argue that their findings are contrary to the general perception of how the basal ganglia operate, which is more centralized, and that their model offers a novel perspective on how the basal ganglia operate.
“Our study indicates that there are potentially multiple neural circuits in the brain that are constantly competing over which action to execute next. This insight is important for understanding more deeply how this system works, which is imperative for treating certain movement disorders, but it goes even further,” Paton said. “Observations from neuroscience are at the core of many machine learning and AI techniques. The idea that decision-making can happen through the interaction of numerous parallel circuits within the same system might prove useful for designing new types of intelligent systems,” he added.
Source: Read Full Article